Effects of Smartphone Activities on Postural Balance in Adolescents with Intellectual Disabilities

96
0
2023-11-16 17:11
MDPI
PTLv2
Followers:3Columns:927

1. Introduction

Adolescents with intellectual disabilities frequently exhibit diminished postural balance, as indicated by a consistent body of research [1,2,3,4,5], which can be attributed to deficits in visual perception, proprioceptive awareness, and vestibular inputs [3]. These deficits significantly impact overall mobility, making it challenging to perform movements and maintain postural balance [3]. Maintaining postural balance is crucial for daily life activities, and it involves the integration of sensory information from various systems in the body, including the visual, proprioceptive, and vestibular systems. However, when adolescents engage in multiple tasks simultaneously, a phenomenon known as dual-tasking postural control [6], their postural balance can be compromised due to divided attention. Previous research has explored dual-task models, particularly in the context of aging and neurodegenerative diseases, shedding light on the intricate interplay between cognitive and motor functions [7]. Furthermore, several studies have demonstrated that individuals with intellectual disabilities, including children and adults, experience alterations in postural balance when simultaneously engaging in supplementary tasks, affecting both static [6,8] and dynamic [9,10,11] balance compared to their counterparts with typical development. However, a noticeable research gap remains concerning the specific exploration of dual-tasking in adolescents with intellectual disabilities.

The rapid rise in smartphone usage, accelerated by the COVID-19 pandemic, has also affected adolescents with intellectual disabilities [12,13]. It has been previously demonstrated that adolescents with intellectual disabilities often participate in various smartphone activities, including playing games, watching videos, listening to music, and engaging in video calls [12]. These activities demand attention, cognitive involvement, and visual concentration, which can potentially influence the postural control system; however, the specific effects of such smartphone activities on the postural balance of adolescents with intellectual disabilities have not been explored yet. In individuals with typical development, research in this area has demonstrated that smartphone use can negatively affect postural balance. For instance, several studies have shown that engaging in smartphone activities, such as texting, browsing, watching videos, etc., lead to postural instability and compromised balance performance [14,15,16,17,18,19].

While the adoption of smartphones among adolescents with intellectual disabilities has provided potential benefits, including enhanced communication, education, and social engagement [12,13], it has also raised concerns about its impact on their physical well-being. Current studies on this topic are limited and inconclusive, making it necessary to conduct comprehensive research to assess the potential risks and benefits associated with smartphone use among this population. Given the motor impairments and cognitive challenges frequently experienced by adolescents with intellectual disabilities, it is essential to investigate the effects of smartphone activities on their postural balance.

Hence, the aim of this study is to investigate the effects of smartphone activities on postural balance in adolescents with intellectual disabilities. By exploring these effects, we aim to gain a better understanding of the challenges faced by this population and develop targeted interventions to optimize balance performance, reduce the risk of falls, and improve overall quality of life. We hypothesize that engaging in smartphone activities, such as playing games, watching videos, listening to music, and engaging in video calls would significantly affect postural balance in adolescents with intellectual disabilities, and that this effect varies depending on the specific activities involved.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample size was a priori calculated using the software G*power for Windows (version 3.1.9.2; Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany). Values for alpha, power, correlation among repeated measures, and non-sphericity correction (ε) were set at 0.05, 0.80, 0.50, and 1, respectively. To reach the desired power, data from at least 12 participants were deemed to be sufficient to minimize the risk of Type II statistical error. To accommodate the possible withdrawal of some participants, we recruited more participants than the number indicated by G*power (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Flow diagram of sample selection and study design.

The present study specifically targeted adolescents with intellectual disabilities who were enrolled in a specialized educational center. To ensure ethical compliance, formal permissions were obtained from the center authorities before initiating data collection. To recruit the sample, a three-stage screening process was employed. In the first stage, 24 adolescents with moderate to mild intellectual disabilities, as determined by the educational center psychologist using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) [20], were randomly selected. For the typical development group (TDG), 19 adolescents without intellectual disability were selected from the nearest secondary school, matching the intellectual disability group (IDG) in terms of age, height, and weight . In the second stage, 18 IDG participants and 14 TDG participants who met the inclusion and exclusion criteria were selected from the screened participants. The common inclusion criteria for both groups were: similar ethnicity, similar social class, low physical activity level (International Physical Activity Questionnaire score < 600 MET), not using neuroleptic medications or other substances that could affect postural balance, no history of lower limb injuries or surgeries within the past year, no visual and/or vestibular disorders. For the IDG, participants had to not use assistive devices for upright stance and walking. In the end, two individuals from each group were excluded from the study, as they were absent during the familiarization session, and the final sample comprised 16 individuals in the IDG and 12 individuals in the TDG (Figure 1).

Prior to initiating the study, a detailed explanation of the experimental protocol was delivered to the participants, parents, and caregivers involved. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents, and assent was sought from the adolescent participants. The assent process involved explaining the study’s purpose, procedures, and potential risks in a developmentally appropriate manner to ensure that the adolescents understood the study and were willing to participate. This study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Vasile Alecsandri University of Bacău Romania (40/2/24 October 2023).

2.2. Study Design

This study employed a crossover comparative design to assess the effect of smartphone activities on static and dynamic balance in IDG and TDG participants. Static postural balance was measured using a stabilometric platform with firm and foam surfaces, while dynamic balance was evaluated using the Timed Up-and-Go Test (TUGT) (Figure 1). These measures were taken in various conditions, such as closed eyes (CE) (only in static postural balance), playing a game, watching videos, calling video, and listening to music. Consistency was maintained across experimental conditions for all participants. The study involved three laboratory visits separated by at least 2 days, with the first focusing on familiarization and the second and third involving the testing sessions in which we investigated the effects of smartphone activities on postural balance.

2.3. Measurements

2.4. Interventions

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed using the program SPSS 25.0 (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to verify the normality of the data distribution.

A three-way (6 Condition × 2 Group × 2 Surface) ANOVA with repeated measures was used to determine the effect of Condition (control/CE/playing game/watching video/video call/listening to music) and/or Group (IDG/TDG) and/or Surface (Firm/Foam) on the CoPVm values. In addition, a two-way (5 Condition × 2 Group) ANOVA with repeated measures was used to determine the effect of Condition (control/CE/playing game/watching video/video call/listening to music) and/or Group (IDG/TDG) on TUGT scores. To evaluate the changes in CoPVm values and TUGT scores after each condition, Δ CoPVm% and TUGT% were calculated using the formula: Δ CoPVm% or TUGT% = 100 × (condition − control)/condition. Subsequently, another two-way ANOVA with repeated measures was carried out to examine the effects of Δ changes in Condition and Group on CoPVm values (control/CE; control/playing game; control/watching video; control/video call; control/listening to music) and TUGT scores (control/playing game; control/watching video; control/video call; control/listening to music). A 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated for means and Δ changes.

To determine whether the statistically significant differences found were practically significant, the effect size of each outcome measure was calculated. The partial eta squared (ηp2) formula was calculated for the main effects and interactions (small: 0.01 < ηp2 < 0.06; moderate: 0.06 < ηp2 < 0.14; large: ηp2 > 0.14), and the Cohen’s d was calculated for the pairwise differences (trivial: d < 0.2; small: 0.2 ≤ d < 0.5; moderate: 0.5 ≤ d < 0.8; large: d ≤ 0.8) [35]. To account for multiple comparisons, a Bonferroni adjustment was conducted. The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Static Balance

3.2. Dynamic Balance

4. Discussion

Our study aimed to investigate the influence of various smartphone activities, including playing video games, watching videos, engaging in video calls, and listening to music, on the static and dynamic postural balance of adolescents with intellectual disabilities, in comparison with their typically developing counterparts. The main findings revealed that the CoPVm values and TUGT scores were significantly deteriorated during various smartphone activities (except listening to music) compared to the control condition in both the IDG and the TDG. Our study findings align with previous research in individuals with intellectual disabilities [8,36,37] as well as with typical development [14,38,39] that have shown impaired performance during dual-tasking in various contexts.

4.1. Static Postural Balance

Our study showed that CoPVm values increased during various activities under both surfaces in the IDG and the TDG. Consistent with previous studies [40,41,42,43], smartphone usage was found to have a detrimental effect on posture, particularly in the neck and trunk regions. These alterations in posture could potentially influence the distribution of plantar pressure and affect stabilometric variables [44], as well as increase postural sway and compromised postural control during smartphone activities [43]. Moreover, our findings highlight the importance of postural control in maintaining stability during these tasks. Postural control is a complex process that involves the integration of sensory information from the visual, vestibular, and somatosensory systems to make continuous adjustments and maintain balance [45]. Engaging in activities that challenge postural control can lead to an increased demand on the sensory systems and motor responses involved in maintaining static postural balance. Indeed, playing games on smartphones involves dynamic movements, body position changes, and rapid shifts in attention and focus. These aspects of gaming require continuous adjustments in body posture and weight distribution to maintain stability and adapt to the game’s demands. Consequently, the visual, vestibular, and somatosensory systems, crucial for postural control, must collaborate effectively to provide accurate feedback and facilitate quick motor responses. Furthermore, playing smartphone games engages cognitive processing and motor skills, making it a multifaceted task that presents various challenges to individuals. Previous studies have indicated that cognitive and postural tasks share common cognitive mechanisms, resulting in potential conflicts when both tasks are performed simultaneously. As a result, postural sway has been shown to increase during cognitive tasks in both individuals with intellectual disabilities [26,36,46] and those without [47]. Moreover, watching videos often requires sustained visual attention, and this visual focus can lead to reduced awareness of one’s body position and balance [48]. As individuals become absorbed in the content of the videos, they may unintentionally neglect their postural adjustments, leading to instability and increased postural sway. Engaging in visual tasks, such as video calls or watching videos, can significantly impact postural control, as individuals tend to rely on their visual input to maintain balance. Therefore, the effect of video on postural control can be attributed to the central role of vision in human perception, as the visual system provides important cues for maintaining stability [49]. In addition, during video calls, individuals often maintain a relatively fixed posture while looking at the screen and interacting with others. This sustained static posture, combined with the cognitive demands of engaging in the conversation, can affect postural stability. Our results are in accordance with previous studies in typical developing individuals demonstrating that the ability to maintain static postural balance decreases while talking on the phone [43,50] or engaging in verbal dual-tasking [51]. Furthermore, the increased instability during video calls could be attributed to the demands of articulation, which involve respiratory activity during speech and may contribute to changes in postural sway, as previously demonstrated [43,52,53]. This is not the case for the listening to music condition, as static postural balance was not affected significantly while using the smartphone to listen to music. This was expected, as this condition does not require participants to divide their attention between dual tasks. Listening to music is a common everyday activity for many individuals and is known to have various effects on human physiology and psychology in different populations [33,34,54,55]. However, its influence on postural balance might not always pronounced, especially in the context of static postural balance. It seems that the specific characteristics of the pop music used in this study might not have provided sufficient cognitive or attentional demands to significantly impact postural stability during the static standing task.

In addition, the study results showed that the significant difference in CoP values between the CE condition and engaging in activities like playing video games, watching videos, and video calls in the IDG underscores the unique challenges faced by individuals with intellectual disabilities in maintaining postural balance. The increased postural sway in the IDG participants during these activities indicates that they struggled to maintain a stable posture compared to the CE condition, where sensory inputs are minimized, and they might rely more on proprioceptive and vestibular inputs. Engaging in activities like playing video games and watching videos involves dynamic visual stimuli, which may disrupt postural stability in individuals with intellectual disabilities. Furthermore, video calls may introduce an additional layer of complexity as they require the integration of both auditory and visual inputs, potentially leading to increased postural sway as the IDG participants focus on answering the questions.

It is important to note that, on a firm surface, this study found that the increase in Δ CoPVm% in response to playing games and participating in video calls was more pronounced in the IDG compared to the TDG. This finding suggests that adolescents with intellectual disabilities may have a more significant challenge in maintaining postural stability while performing these activities compared to typically developing adolescents. Indeed, individuals with intellectual disabilities may have differences in sensory processing, which could affect how they perceive and respond to sensory cues related to balance and postural control [3]. It seems that playing games and engaging in video calls, which involve visually complex and dynamic stimuli, as well as multitasking and attention to both visual and auditory stimuli, may pose greater challenges for individuals with intellectual disabilities. These cognitive demands might lead to increased postural sway and difficulties in maintaining static postural balance during these activities. Furthermore, variability in motor skills, attention, and cognitive abilities among participants in the IDG [56] may have contributed to the observed differences in postural sway [3,5,57]. In our study, we observed that participants encountered considerable challenges in dividing their attention between dual tasks during the playing game and video call conditions. These conditions appeared to be more demanding, potentially leading to increased difficulty and inducing fatigue in the postural muscles [58] responsible for maintaining balance.

The analysis of Δ CoPVm% on the foam surface revealed that there were no significant differences in postural sway between the IDG and the TDG across all conditions studied. This finding suggests that the foam surface, which provides an unstable and challenging base of support, had a similar impact on static postural balance for both groups. In fact, the foam surface is known to elicit greater postural instability by reducing the proprioceptive and tactile feedback from the feet [59,60]. This sensory alteration creates a more demanding postural control task for individuals, regardless of their intellectual ability. It seems that both groups may have faced comparable challenges in adapting to the unstable foam surface, resulting in similar postural responses.

4.2. Dynamic Balance

Concerning dynamic balance, similar results were observed in the effect of smartphone activities. Playing video games, watching videos, and engaging in video calls led to an increase in TUGT scores in both the IDG and the TDG compared to the control condition. These findings align with previous studies that have reported impaired walking during dual-tasking activities and smartphone use [17,18]. In fact, previous research has demonstrated that engaging in dual-task activities on a smartphone while walking can negatively affect working memory and lead to increased errors, including greater lateral deviations in walking [61]. Another study investigating ground walking mechanics and speed during phone use found that as the complexity of the task increased, the detrimental impact on gait mechanics worsened [62]. These findings highlight the potential risks and challenges associated with using a smartphone while walking, particularly in situations that demand cognitive attention and motor coordination. In our study, it seems that engaging in smartphone activities, such as playing games, watching videos, or participating in video calls, may challenge dynamic balance due to potential divided visual attention and cognitive overload. Participants may have faced difficulties in effectively allocating attention between the video call and the motor task of walking, ultimately affecting their dynamic balance during these activities. Significantly, these results show a more pronounced increase in Δ TUGT% among IDG participants compared to TDG participants. This finding aligns with a previous study involving older adults with Parkinson’s disease, where, when presented with a dual task, they demonstrated a significant reduction in walking speed compared to older adults without Parkinson’s disease [7]. In both cases, the dual-task scenario and the shared impact on dynamic balance underscore the common challenges individuals with specific conditions face when multitasking during walking. In our study, adolescents with intellectual disabilities may experience challenges in attentional control and divided attention [63], making it more difficult for them to allocate their cognitive resources effectively while engaging in such activities, specifically during dynamic balance tasks. This could lead to compromised performance in dynamic balance assessments for the IDG compared to the TDG during these activities.

It is important to note that a unique result emerged in the listening to music condition, where both groups exhibited improved dynamic balance. Participants might have experienced a positive emotional response while listening to music, which could have led to increased confidence and focus [64,65] during the TUGT, contributing to better dynamic balance performance. Music is known to have a profound impact on emotions and mood. In our study we selected pop music with fast tempo. This music genre is widely recognized for its lively and catchy tunes, rhythmic beats, and upbeat melodies [66]. These musical characteristics often evoke positive emotions, energy, and a sense of excitement in listeners [67]. As a result, when individuals are exposed to pop music, they may feel more inclined to move and engage in physical activities [66]. In this context, it seems that the use of pop music with fast tempo could have influenced participants’ movement behaviors during the timed tasks. The energetic and vibrant nature of such genre of music might have inspired participants to adopt a more active and dynamic gait pattern during the walking assessments, such as the TUGT. Importantly, our findings suggest that even brief music exposure can lead to immediate dynamic balance enhancements. Moreover, it has been indicated that music may influence autonomic nervous system activity [68], which could contribute to these acute balance improvements. The influence of different of music genre on postural balance and chronic effects of music should be investigated in future studies.

4.3. Strengths and Limitations

This study was the first of its kind to investigate the effects of various smartphone activities on both static and dynamic postural balance in adolescents with intellectual disabilities compared to their typically developing counterparts. The unique focus on smartphone activities provided valuable insights into a relatively unexplored area of research. Moreover, this study assessed a diverse range of smartphone activities, including playing games, watching videos, and engaging in video calls. By examining multiple activities, this study captured a comprehensive understanding of their impact on postural balance. Furthermore, by comparing the results between adolescents with intellectual disabilities and their typically developing counterparts, this study highlighted potential differences in how smartphone activities affect postural balance in these two groups.

Despite the valuable insights gained from our study, several limitations should be acknowledged. While our study’s controlled experimental conditions provided valuable insights, they may not fully capture the complexities of real-world mobile device use, potentially restricting the generalizability of our findings to everyday situations. Future research endeavors should consider conducting investigations in more naturalistic settings, encompassing the dynamic and multifaceted nature of mobile device utilization in daily life, to gain a deeper understanding of its impact on postural balance. In addition, we did not explore the possibility that augmentation of postural sway might, in certain circumstances, represent a positive adaptation to task constraints, affording individuals greater degrees of freedom for dynamic postural adjustments. We also did not employ non-linear methods to investigate the potential benefits of increased noise in postural responses. This limitation underscores the need for future research to delve deeper into these nuances and expand our understanding of postural balance. Moreover, this study primarily focused on static and dynamic postural balance assessments. While these are valuable indicators of postural balance, other factors such as muscle strength and joint stability were not extensively examined. Future research could incorporate a more comprehensive assessment of various aspects of postural balance to gain a greater understanding of the relationship between smartphone activities and postural balance. Likewise, while our study focused on the postural balance task while listening to music, it did not investigate the concept of a “resonant effect” induced by music as well as other variables, such as the type of music, tempo, or individual differences in music preferences, which could have significant effects on postural balance. Future research to include these factors would offer a more comprehensive understanding of the influence of music on postural balance. Furthermore, this study did not investigate the potential long-term effects of smartphone activities on postural balance. This would be essential for considering the broader implications for individuals with intellectual disabilities and typically developing individuals.

4.4. Practical Implications

The findings of this study hold significant practical implications for adolescents with intellectual disabilities, healthcare professionals, and caregivers alike. In fact, educating caregivers about the potential effects of smartphone activities on postural balance is crucial. By raising awareness and providing guidance on responsible smartphone use, caregivers can play a vital role in ensuring the safety and well-being of adolescents with intellectual disabilities. In addition, collaboration between healthcare professionals, educators, therapists, and technology experts can lead to the development of innovative solutions and interventions to enhance postural balance during smartphone activities. Long-term monitoring is also recommended to track changes in postural control and balance over time. By regularly assessing postural balance, healthcare professionals can identify trends and implement timely interventions, contributing to better outcomes for individuals with intellectual disabilities. These practical measures align with the broader goal of preventing and reducing the risk of falls, ultimately enhancing the quality of life for adolescents with intellectual disabilities.

5. Conclusions

This study highlighted that engaging in smartphone activities, including playing video games, watching videos, and participating in video calls, has significant effects on both static and dynamic postural balance in both groups. These activities present challenges to postural balance and can lead to increased postural sway, affecting the performance during tasks like the TUGT. Moreover, adolescents with intellectual disabilities demonstrated more pronounced alterations in postural balance in response to playing games and video call smartphone activities compared to their typically developing counterparts. This highlighted the unique challenges that smartphone interactions pose for adolescents with intellectual disabilities, with cognitive and attentional demands potentially playing a crucial role in affecting their postural balance during these tasks. In addition, the finding that listening to music led to increased dynamic balance scores in both groups raises interesting questions about the influence of music genre on postural responses. Future research could explore the specific effects of different music genres on postural control to gain a deeper understanding of this phenomenon. In practical term, our study highlights the need for assistive devices and caregiver education to improve smartphone usage safety for adolescents with intellectual disabilities. Collaboration between healthcare professionals, educators, therapists, and technology experts can lead to the development of innovative solutions and interventions to enhance postural balance during smartphone activities in order to prevent and reduce the risk of falls, ultimately enhancing individuals’ quality of life.

References

  1. Cabeza-Ruiz, R.; García-Massó, X.; Centeno-Prada, R.; Beas-Jiménez, J.; Colado, J.; González, L.-M. Time and frequency analysis of the static balance in young adults with Down syndrome. Gait Posture 2011, 33, 23–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Carmeli, E.; Barchad, S.; Lenger, R.; Coleman, R. Muscle power, locomotor performance and flexibility in aging mentally-retarded adults with and without Down’s syndrome. J. Musculoskelet. Neuronal Interact. 2002, 2, 457–462. [Google Scholar]
  3. Leyssens, L.; Van Hecke, R.; Moons, K.; Luypaert, S.; Danneels, M.; Patru, J.; Willems, M.; Maes, L. Postural balance problems in people with intellectual disabilities: Do not forget the sensory input systems. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. Disabil. 2022, 35, 280–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Chiba, Y.; Shimada, A.; Yoshida, F.; Keino, H.; Hasegawa, M.; Ikari, H.; Miyake, S.; Hosokawa, M. Risk of fall for individuals with intellectual disability. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2009, 114, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Blomqvist, S.; Olsson, J.; Wallin, L.; Wester, A.; Rehn, B. Adolescents with intellectual disability have reduced postural balance and muscle performance in trunk and lower limbs compared to peers without intellectual disability. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 198–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kachouri, H.; Jouira, G.; Laatar, R.; Borji, R.; Rebai, H.; Sahli, S. Different types of combined training programs to improve postural balance in single and dual tasks in children with intellectual disability. J. Intellect. Disabil. 2022, 17446295221148585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Ivaniski-Mello, A.; Müller, V.T.; de Liz Alves, L.; Casal, M.Z.; Haas, A.N.; Correale, L.; Kanitz, A.C.; Martins, V.F.; Gonçalves, A.K.; Martinez, F.G. Determinants of dual-task gait speed in older adults with and without Parkinson’s disease. Int. J. Sport. Med. 2023, 44, 744–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Pena, G.; Pavão, S.; Oliveira, M.; Godoi, D.; De Campos, A.; Rocha, N. Dual-task effects on postural sway during sit-to-stand movement in children with Down syndrome. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2019, 63, 576–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Horvat, M.; Croce, R.; Tomporowski, P.; Barna, M. The influence of dual-task conditions on movement in young adults with and without Down syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 3517–3525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Holfelder, B.; Klotzbier, T.J.; Schott, N. Dual-task interference in children with Down syndrome and chronological and mental age-matched healthy controls. Children 2022, 9, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Oppewal, A.; Hilgenkamp, T.I. The dual task effect on gait in adults with intellectual disabilities: Is it predictive for falls? Disabil. Rehabil. 2019, 41, 26–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Arun, P.; Jain, S. Use of smart phone among students with intellectual and developmental disability. J. Psychosoc. Rehabil. Ment. Health 2022, 9, 447–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lancioni, G.E.; Singh, N.N.; O’Reilly, M.F.; Sigafoos, J.; Alberti, G.; Zimbaro, C.; Chiariello, V. Using smartphones to help people with intellectual and sensory disabilities perform daily activities. Front. Public Health 2017, 5, 282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Rebold, M.J.; Croall, C.A.; Cumberledge, E.A.; Sheehan, T.P.; Dirlam, M.T. The impact of different cell phone functions and their effects on postural stability. Perform. Enhanc. Health 2017, 5, 98–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. De Freitas Brandão, A.; Palluel, E.; Olivier, I.; Nougier, V. Effects of emotional videos on postural control in children. Gait Posture 2016, 45, 175–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Cho, S.-H.; Choi, M.-H.; Goo, B.-O. Effect of smart phone use on dynamic postural balance. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 2014, 26, 1013–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Lee, J.H.; Lee, M.H. The effects of smartphone multitasking on gait and dynamic balance. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 2018, 30, 293–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lino, T.B.; Scarmagnan, G.S.; Sobrinho-Junior, S.A.; Tessari, G.M.; Gonçalves, G.H.; Pereira, H.M.; Christofoletti, G. Impact of Using Smartphone While Walking or Standing: A Study Focused on Age and Cognition. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Lee, D.; Hong, S.; Jung, S.; Lee, K.; Lee, G. The effects of viewing smart devices on static balance, oculomotor function, and dizziness in healthy adults. Med. Sci. Monit. Int. Med. J. Exp. Clin. Res. 2019, 25, 8055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th ed.; WISC-IV; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003; Available online: https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1370567187560684295 (accessed on 24 October 2023).
  21. Borji, R.; Laatar, R.; Zarrouk, N.; Sahli, S.; Rebai, H. Cognitive-motor interference during standing stance across different postural and cognitive tasks in individuals with Down syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2023, 139, 104562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Jouira, G.; Srihi, S.; Kachouri, H.; Ben Waer, F.; Rebai, H.; Sahli, S. Static postural balance between male athletes with intellectual disabilities and their sedentary peers: A comparative study. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. Disabil. 2021, 34, 1136–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Hiller, C.E.; Nightingale, E.J.; Lin, C.-W.C.; Coughlan, G.F.; Caulfield, B.; Delahunt, E. Characteristics of people with recurrent ankle sprains: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sport. Med. 2011, 45, 660–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Paillard, T.; Noé, F. Techniques and methods for testing the postural function in healthy and pathological subjects. BioMed Res. Int. 2015, 2015, 891390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Beerse, M.; Lelko, M.; Wu, J. Biomechanical analysis of the timed up-and-go (TUG) test in children with and without Down syndrome. Gait Posture 2019, 68, 409–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kachouri, H.; Laatar, R.; Borji, R.; Rebai, H.; Sahli, S. Using a dual-task paradigm to investigate motor and cognitive performance in children with intellectual disability. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. Disabil. 2020, 33, 172–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Jouira, G.; Srihi, S.; Ben Waer, F.; Rebai, H.; Sahli, S. Comparison of Dynamic Balance Performances of Adult Runners With Intellectual Disability Versus Their Sedentary Peers. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2022, 129, 1443–1457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Nobakht, F.; Mirmahdi, R.; Heidari, H. Effect of computer games (puzzle game and Simulation game) in working memory and space visual perception in student with specific learning disorder (reading, writing, math). Arch. Pharm. Pract. 2020, 1, 55. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kogan, L.R.; Hellyer, P.W.; Clapp, T.R.; Suchman, E.; McLean, J.; Schoenfeld-Tacher, R. Use of short animal-themed videos to enhance veterinary students’ mood, attention, and understanding of pharmacology lectures. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 2018, 45, 188–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Lavan, S.; Ein, N.; Vickers, K. Examining the Effects of Nature and Animal Videos on Stress. Anthrozoös 2023, 36, 849–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kitaoka, K.; Ito, R.; Araki, H.; Sei, H.; Morita, Y. Effect of mood state on anticipatory postural adjustments. Neurosci. Lett. 2004, 370, 65–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Lancioni, G.E.; O’Reilly, M.F.; Sigafoos, J.; Alberti, G.; Chiariello, V.; Resta, E. A Smartphone-Aided Program to Support Video Calls, Leisure, and Occupational Activities in People with Moderate Intellectual Disability. Adv. Neurodev. Disord. 2020, 4, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ruscello, B.; D’ottavio, S.; Padua, E.; Tonelli, C.; Pantanella, L. The influence of music on exercise in a group of sedentary elderly women: An important tool to help the elderly to stay active. J. Sport. Med. Phys. Fit. 2014, 54, 536–544. [Google Scholar]
  34. Terry, P.C.; Karageorghis, C.I.; Saha, A.M.; D’Auria, S. Effects of synchronous music on treadmill running among elite triathletes. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2012, 15, 52–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  36. Laatar, R.; Kachouri, H.; Borji, R.; Ben Waer, F.; Rebai, H.; Sahli, S. Dual-task affects postural balance performance in children with intellectual disability. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2023, 40, 33–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Atak, E.; Hajebrahimi, F.; Algun, Z.C. The effect of Dual-Task balance exercises on cognitive functions among children with mild and borderline mental retardation: A randomized controlled trial. Eur. J. Physiother. 2022, 24, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Rebold, M.J.; Lepp, A.; Sanders, G.J.; Barkley, J.E. The impact of cell phone use on the intensity and liking of a bout of treadmill exercise. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0125029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Fennell, C.; Barkley, J.E.; Lepp, A. The relationship between cell phone use, physical activity, and sedentary behavior in adults aged 18–80. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 90, 53–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Park, J.-H.; Kang, S.-Y.; Lee, S.-G.; Jeon, H.-S. The effects of smart phone gaming duration on muscle activation and spinal posture: Pilot study. Physiother. Theory Pract. 2017, 33, 661–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Cochrane, M.E.; Tshabalala, M.D.; Hlatswayo, N.C.; Modipana, R.M.; Makibelo, P.P.; Mashale, E.P.; Pete, L.C. The short-term effect of smartphone usage on the upper-back postures of university students. Cogent Eng. 2019, 6, 1627752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lee, S.-Y.; Lee, D.-H.; Han, S.-K. The effects of posture on neck flexion angle while using a smartphone according to duration. Korean Soc. Phys. Med. 2016, 11, 35–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Onofrei, R.R.; Amaricai, E.; Suciu, O.; David, V.L.; Rata, A.L.; Hogea, E. Smartphone use and postural balance in healthy young adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Szczygieł, E.; Piotrowski, K.; Golec, J.; Czechowska, D.; Masłoń, A.; Bac, A.; Golec, E. Head position influence on stabilographic variables. Acta Bioeng. Biomech. 2016, 18, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  45. Ivanenko, Y.; Gurfinkel, V.S. Human postural control. Front. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Van Biesen, D.; Jacobs, L.; McCulloch, K.; Janssens, L.; Vanlandewijck, Y.C. Cognitive-motor dual-task ability of athletes with and without intellectual impairment. J. Sport. Sci. 2018, 36, 513–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Kim, T.; Jimenez-Diaz, J.; Chen, J. The effect of attentional focus in balancing tasks: A systematic review with meta-analysis. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 2017, 12, 463–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lim, Y.; Kim, J.; Lee, H.; Kim, S. Postural instability induced by visual motion stimuli in patients with vestibular migraine. Front. Neurol. 2018, 9, 433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Luo, H.; Wang, X.; Fan, M.; Deng, L.; Jian, C.; Wei, M.; Luo, J. The effect of visual stimuli on stability and complexity of postural control. Front. Neurol. 2018, 9, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Laatar, R.; Kachouri, H.; Borji, R.; Rebai, H.; Sahli, S. The effect of cell phone use on postural balance and mobility in older compared to young adults. Physiol. Behav. 2017, 173, 293–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hsiao, D.; Belur, P.; Myers, P.S.; Earhart, G.M.; Rawson, K.S. The impact of age, surface characteristics, and dual-tasking on postural sway. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2020, 87, 103973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Yardley, L.; Gardner, M.; Leadbetter, A.; Lavie, N. Effect of articulatory and mental tasks on postural control. Neuroreport 1999, 10, 215–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Dault, M.C.; Yardley, L.; Frank, J.S. Does articulation contribute to modifications of postural control during dual-task paradigms? Cogn. Brain Res. 2003, 16, 434–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Xu, H.; Ohgami, N.; He, T.; Hashimoto, K.; Tazaki, A.; Ohgami, K.; Takeda, K.; Kato, M. Improvement of balance in young adults by a sound component at 100 Hz in music. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 16894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Maatoug, H.; Baccouch, R.; Borji, R.; Rebai, H.; Sahli, S. Effects of music listening on postural balance in adolescents with visual impairment. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2023, 130, 112–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Boat, T.F.; Wu, J.T.; National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Clinical characteristics of intellectual disabilities. In Mental Disorders and Disabilities among Low-Income Children; National Academies Press (US): Washington, DC, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  57. Rintala, P.; Loovis, E.M. Measuring motor skills in Finnish children with intellectual disabilities. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2013, 116, 294–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Tsuchida, W.; Nakagawa, K.; Kawahara, Y.; Yuge, L. Influence of dual-task performance on muscle and brain activity. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 2013, 36, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Chiang, J.-H.; Wu, G. The influence of foam surfaces on biomechanical variables contributing to postural control. Gait Posture 1997, 5, 239–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Patel, M.; Fransson, P.-A.; Lush, D.; Gomez, S. The effect of foam surface properties on postural stability assessment while standing. Gait Posture 2008, 28, 649–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Lamberg, E.M.; Muratori, L.M. Cell phones change the way we walk. Gait Posture 2012, 35, 688–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Parr, N.D.; Hass, C.J.; Tillman, M.D. Cellular phone texting impairs gait in able-bodied young adults. J. Appl. Biomech. 2014, 30, 685–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Spaniol, M.; Danielsson, H. A meta-analysis of the executive function components inhibition, shifting, and attention in intellectual disabilities. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2022, 66, 9–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Shayan, N.; AhmadiGatab, T.; Jeloudar, J.G.; Ahangar, K.S. The effect of playing music on the confidence level. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 30, 2061–2063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lanzillo, J.J. The Effects of Music on the Intensity and Direction of Pre-Competitive Cognitive and Somatic State Anxiety and State Self-Confidence in Collegiate Athletes. Master’s Thesis, Georgia Southern University, Statesboro, GA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  66. Khan, M.; Ajmal, A. Effect of classical and pop music on mood and performance. Int. J. Sci. Res. Publ. 2017, 7, 905–911. [Google Scholar]
  67. Juslin, P.N.; Sloboda, J.A. Music and emotion. In The Psychology of Music; Deutsch, D., Ed.; Elsevier Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013; pp. 583–645. [Google Scholar]
  68. McCrary, J.M.; Altenmüller, E. Mechanisms of music impact: Autonomic tone and the physical activity roadmap to advancing understanding and evidence-based policy. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 727231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Related Suggestion
Study on the Mechanism of Influencing Adolescents’ Willingness to Participate in Ice Sports
Clinical
Background: Ice sports are an effective means to promote the physical health of adolescents, and the willingness to participate in ice sports is the basis of adolescents’ awareness of their participation in ice sports and an important prerequisite for their participation. The aim of this study is to investigate the mechanisms influencing adolescents’ willingness to participate in ice sports. Methods: Using the stimulus-organism-response theoretical framework, the motivation-opportunity-ability model, and the technology acceptance model, a theoretical model of the influencing mechanisms of adolescents’ participation in ice sports was constructed. A total of 3419 secondary school students were surveyed, and the data were analyzed using structural equation modeling, the mechanisms influencing adolescents’ willingness to participate in ice sports and the moderating role of perceived riskiness and perceived ease of use are proposed and tested. Results: The study found that participation motivation, participation ability, perceived usefulness, and perceived risk all have significant effects on adolescents’ willingness to participate in ice sports. The degree of influence in descending order is as follows: ability to participate, perceived usefulness, motivation to participate, and perceived risk. Perceived risk plays a mediating role in the process of motivation to participate and ability to participate influencing willingness to participate. Perceived ease of use plays a mediating role in the process of motivation to participate influencing perceived usefulness. Conclusions: This study provides a systematic understanding of the mechanisms that influence adolescents’ willingness to participate in ice sports. The findings provide valuable insights into the subject of adolescents’ ice sport participation and can inform the development of strategies to increase participation in these activities. Future research should deepen the investigation of the patterns of willingness to participate in ice sports behavior among adole
35
0
Stress and Anxiety among High School Adolescents: Correlations between Physiological and Psychological Indicators in a Longitudinal Follow-Up Study
Clinical
Mental and psychological disorders are serious health problems worldwide. Anxiety among high school students can affect school performance, relationships, and family life. Objective: Our aim is to understand the anxiety levels and associated factors among high school students and compare the results of psychological tests measuring anxiety with the cortisol levels obtained from biological sampling. Method: In our longitudinal follow-up study, we involved 125 individuals in May 2019. Validated measurement tools were used during questionnaire data collection, including the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory, Clear Communication Scale, Multiple Social Perceived Support Scale, and related HBSC questions. As objective data, we collected hair samples for cortisol level measurement. Results: At the end of the school year, the anxiety levels measured by psychological tests were significantly higher (p = 0.001) compared to the anxiety levels at the beginning of the next school year. Anxiety levels were higher among girls and were influenced by the type of school and parental expectations. Both state anxiety and trait anxiety showed a strong correlation with psychosomatic symptoms (p < 0.001) and anxiety arising from school expectations (p < 0.05). The changes in cortisol levels did not follow the changes in psychological tests. Cortisol level increased (p = 0.01) in the second sample.
24
0
Comments 0
Please to post a comment~
Loading...
Likes
Send-Pen
Favorites
Comment