Audiogenic Seizures and Social Deficits: No Aggravation Found in Krushinsky–Molodkina Rats

45
0
2023-9-19 16:49
MDPI
PTLv2
Followers:3Columns:927

1. Introduction

Epilepsy and epileptic syndromes are among the main neurological comorbidities of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). They are diagnosed in 7% of autistic children and in as many as 19% of autistic adults [1]. Seizure history is reported as a risk factor for intellectual disabilities and social regresses [2,3,4]. Experience of seizure is thought to provoke hidden epileptogenesis, rewiring the brain and lowering the threshold for the next convulsions.

Since there is a significant rate of the ASD/epilepsy comorbidity in human patients [5,6], ASD-like phenotypes should also be more expectable in epilepsy-prone laboratory animals, if compared to the entity of seizure-resistant ones. So, to find an animal model for ASD, it is reasonable to screen animal epilepsy models for ASD-like phenotypes. A possibility to assess putative epilepsy-related effects on social deficits would bring an advantage of accumulated epileptology knowledge to a relatively new field of neurology of ASD.

Indeed, there are several animal models of epilepsy with reported social deficits [6,7,8,9,10]. On the other hand, the connections between ASD and epilepsy are neither unidirectional nor simple, both in clinical practice and basic research. In a number of animal epilepsy models [11,12,13,14], minor or no deficits in conspecific communications were registered. In a mouse model [15], ASD-like and epilepsy-like behaviors did not demonstrate any additivity, but rather counteracted. In clinics, prescriptions of antiepileptic drugs do not help to mitigate ASD phenotypes, but might even worsen the behavioral adaptation of patients [16]. So, ASD-like behavioral phenotypes might not be directly caused by seizure experience, but may partly share common neuropathological mechanisms. For a stepwise experimental approach to study the ASD/epilepsy interaction, one needs to provoke seizure fits according to a controlled schedule. Animal models are indispensable for such experimentations.

Recently, we reported social contact deficits in rats of Krushinsky–Molodkina (KM) strain [17]. This Wistar-derived inbred [18] strain has been used as a model for human temporal lobe epilepsy and vascular pathology from the late 1940s [19] to currently [20,21,22,23]. All KM rats are susceptible to audiogenic seizures (AGS) of the maximal score, in response to sound provocations. The seizures fits start from a paroxysmal running and quickly proceed to the clonic and tonic-clonic seizures, followed by profound postictal catalepsy. Importantly, generalized seizures can be triggered in drug-naïve animals. It leaves out the question of possible focal effects, as well as side effects of chemical convulsants, and allows for an unbiased investigation of post-convulsive behavior.

Seizure-naïve KM rats demonstrated social contact deficits, as we reported previously [17]. Namely, KM rats developed a highly exaggerated freezing response to a presence of social stimuli, spent less time in a close proximity to unfamiliar conspecifics and demonstrated a lower number of contacts, as compared to control Wistar rats. It is not known whether a start of seizure would further aggravate social deficits in these epileptic rats. We enrolled a battery of sociability tests, to study the very first stage of ASD/epilepsy interaction: behavioral consequences of rare, generalized tonic-clonic seizure in drug-naive KM rats. The test paradigms varied in spatial design and also enrolled different rats as the social stimuli, to level out possible influences of individual traits of “guest” animals, as well as of spatial context. In the rat, as well as in other species, social interactions of conspecifics are highly dependent on gender, feasibility of aggressive actions, sensory modalities available for interaction, dominant ranks of interacting animals, presence of a shelter, and general available space. Although we did not aim to study gender effects here, the other factors listed above were varied in our experimental routine. Both spatial conditions available for interaction of conspecifics and sufficient time of experiments were considered as important factors. The battery of behavioral tests used also allowed us to compare the obtained results with literature data, since there is no single and commonly accepted set-up for sociability tests in laboratory animals yet [24].

The relatively mild scheme of seizure fits provocation, used in the present experiments, let us keep focused on the beginning of the active epileptogenesis and assess its effect on social contact motivation. The schedule of rare seizures was chosen to escape kindling, which would recruit new neuronal loops and hyper-activate limbic structures [25]. Limbic activation would in turn trigger its own behavioral consequences [26]. As it was shown recently, a fourteen-day schedule of AGS kindling in KM rats led to evident neurodegenerative processes, such as an irreversible hippocampal loss of granular and hilar mossy cells, as well as a reduced proliferation of inferior colliculus neurons [27]. In a shorter scheme, seven-day AGS provocation design, the cell losses were rescued by autophagy [27]. An overspread of epileptiform activity in the magnocellular neurons of the hypothalamus, induced by audiogenic kindling, evoked neuroendocrine changes in the hypothalamic-neurohypophysial system [28]. So, frequent and numerous seizure fits provocations produce a complex response, which needs to be dissected and analyzed separately in further experimentations.

2. Materials and Methods

There is an increasing attention to experimental studies of sociability in laboratory animals. No single experimental design is commonly accepted, but a variety of non-standardized tests are used [24,29]. Social interaction might be assessed in a couple of freely moving animals, placed in a simple or enriched context [12,29,30,31]. Alternatively, “guest” animals might be fixed with tether and collar assemblies [30,32], separated in a compartment of the test arena [33,34,35], or encaged [36,37,38]. The social stimuli might be isolated by partitions, which specifically limit or restrict visual, audial, haptic, or olfactory sensory inputs involved in animal interactions [39,40,41]. Assessment of behavioral parameters in freely moving pairs of animals represented the most difficult case for automated tracking, but the obstacle seems to be resolved soon with algorithms of machine learning [29]. Currently, social interaction of two freely moving animals in laboratory conditions implies a high amount of tracking artifacts. A disadvantage of this design is a possibility of aggressive actions for the experimental animals. More frequently, experimenters let the test animal choose locomotor strategies freely, whereas the social stimuli are localized in a compartment or encagement. This limits behavioral repertoire of social stimuli, but not the test rodents. The locomotion of test animals might be tracked automatically, with a possibility to select virtual zones of interest. Usually, conventional trackers allow an automated detection of freezing behavior as well. Nowadays, automated tracking is usually complemented with offline semi-automated detection of contact as well as investigatory and comfort behavioral elements, performed by blinded experts. For more details on social tests in laboratory rodents, a reader is referred to the recent reviews [24,29,42].

We chose a battery of behavioral tests for sociability (“the socially enriched open field”, “three chambered social preference/social novelty tests”, “two choice social preference/social novelty tests”, see Section 2.3 and Section 2.4), to detect intricacies of conspecific contacts in a robust way. The use of multiple tests minimized possible artifacts related to spatial context [29], as well as a putative bias related to individuality of social stimuli. The experimental schemes described below (Section 2.3 and Section 2.4) assumed the free movement of experimental animals so that they could adjust the intensity of social approaches to encaged unfamiliar conspecifics.

2.1. Animals

The experiments were performed in 43 KM and 44 Wistar male rats in total. The animals were 5 months old at the start of experimentation and weighed 300–350 g. The animals were housed 3–5 in a cage, with standard pellet food and water ad lib. Behavioral tests were scheduled with a 6–10 days interval between the manipulations, unless otherwise stated.

The main behavioral tests were performed in two rounds, pre- and post-provocation of AGS.

After the 1st set of the tests (i.e., the “elevated plus maze”, “socially enriched open field”, “social preference/social novelty”), sound provocations were scheduled for half of the experimental animals. For this purpose, the rats were randomly assigned to the “sound provocation subgroup” (n = 8 for both strains) and the “sham provocation” subgroup (n = 8 for both strains). The sound provocation/sham was performed in 3 sessions, separated by 6–8 days. The sound administration induced audiogenic seizure fits in all KM rats, but not in any of the Wistar rats, as described below (Section 2.6). In a week after the last sound stress, the main behavioral tests (i.e., the “elevated plus maze”, “socially enriched open field”, “social preference/social novelty”) were repeated.

Additional tests (“two choice social preference/social novelty”, “social dominance tube test”, “novel object recognition test”) were run in the animal cohorts, not subjected to the sound stress.

2.2. Elevated Plus Maze

A standard “elevated plus maze” test (EPM) was performed, as described in [43]. In short, the apparatus consisted of 2 open arms (10 cm × 46 cm), 2 closed arms (10 cm × 46 cm × 16 cm), and a central plate (10 cm × 10 cm). The maze was raised 90 cm from the floor surface. Video camera was installed about 120 cm above the maze. The experiment was recorded using a computer in the next room.

The rats (n = 16 for each strain) were placed individually on a central plate, pointing its head to the open arm and turning their backs to the experimenter; landing served as a signal to start tracking. The animals were allowed to explore the maze for 10 min. The video archives were analyzed off-line, as described below (see Section 2.7). The time spent and distance travelled in open and closed arms, rearing, short (facial) and long (whole body) grooming, stereotyping (if any), defecation and urination (if any) were counted.

2.3. Sociability Tests

The rat cohorts were tested in different spatial modifications of the social preference tests, to compare the results with literature data. The main idea of the social tests was to allow a freely moving test animal to explore the available arena with an encaged unfamiliar conspecific. As it was shown previously, normal rats demonstrate a clear preference to stay in a neighborhood of sheltered newcomer rats, rather than nearby empty cages [24,34,35].

2.4. Social Dominance Test

The social dominance tube test (TSD) was carried out using a 150 cm-long transparent plexiglass tube with an internal diameter of 6 cm [45]. The experiment was carried out for three days: on the first two days, the animals were allowed to explore a new environment and pass through the pipe in both directions, on the third day, the testing itself was carried out. Prior to the experiment, the rats were weighed and assigned to pairs so that in each of them were unfamiliar Wistar and KM male rats of a similar body weight. The rats were placed at the opposite ends of the pipe (Figure 1(d.1)) and allowed to move (Figure 1(d.2)). The “winner” was considered the animal that was able to push the opponent back and exit from the opposite end of the tube (Figure 1(d.3)). Subsequently, the video recording of the experiment was analyzed. We recorded the latency to wins, the number of pushes, forward propulsions, and retreats [46].

Figure 1. Schemes of experimental set-ups used in the study. (a.1) Three-chambered social preference test; (a.2) Three-chambered social novelty test; (b.1) The two-choice test for social preference/social novelty, 1st session, social preference; (b.2) The two-choice test for social preference/social novelty, 2nd session, social novelty (c) Socially enriched open field; (d.1d.3) different stages of social dominance test; (e.1,e.2) 1st and 2nd session of two-object/novel object recognition test.

The experimental groups consisted of 20 KM males and 16 Wistar rats, not subjected to the AGS provocations.

2.5. Two-Object Novel Object Recognition

The Novel Object Recognition [47] task was conducted in an open field arena (60 × 60 × 60 cm, the same apparatus as described above at Section 2.3.3, but without the partitions), supplemented with two different kinds of new objects (plastic toy bricks sized as 5 × 5 × 8 cm). Both objects were similar in height and volume, but different in shape and appearance. During 15 min of habituation, the animals were allowed to explore the empty arena. One hour after habituation, the animals were exposed to the familiar arena with two identical objects placed at an equal distance. Next, the test rat was allowed to explore the open field in a presence of the familiar object and a novel object to test recognition memory. Each of the sessions lasted 10 min, instead of the standard 3 min, to account for the hypolocomotion of KM rats. In addition to the standard indicators described above, the following were also registered: contacts with the object—sniffing the object and active contact with the object—touching and moving the object with front paws, turning the object over, and transferring in the teeth.

The experimental groups consisted of 12 KM males and 12 Wistar rats, not subjected to the AGS provocations.

2.6. AGS Provocation

After the 1st set of behavioral tests, a sound provocation scheme [48,49,50,51] was randomly assigned to a half of KM and Wistar cohorts. Briefly, individual rats were placed in a sound-attenuated box (47 × 47 × 47 cm) and exposed to a loud (80–120 dB) sound (electric bell ringing) for 90 s, or until the tonic phase of the seizure was reached (score 9 according to the scale proposed by P. Jobe [52]). The sound evoked initial freezing and then an orientation response in Wistar rats. All KM individuals developed the fully blown AGS (Figure 2) with a latency <15 s. The sham-provocations were the placement into the same box without a sound exposure.

Figure 2. The stages of audiogenic fits in KM rats. The seizure fits start with motor excitation (wild running with a loss of visual control), quickly proceed to clonic convulsions on a side and on the belly, and end up with tonic phases (with the tonic limbs’ extension). These stages of audiogenic fits are: wild run, clonic seizures on a belly, tonic seizure. In total, 100% of the KM rats develop the maximal stage of audiogenic fits in response to the sound administration.

The triple AGS provocations or sham-provocations were scheduled in a week interval, to escape kindling of the seizures (see Introduction). In a week post-provocations, the “elevated plus maze” test (Section 2.2) and the main sociability tests (Section 2.3.2 and Section 2.3.3) were repeated.

2.7. Automated Tracking

All sessions were recorded on video using a digital camera connected to a computer in the next room. The recordings were digitized and analyzed automatically off-line (track length, freezing, time spent in virtually defined marked areas) and off-line by blind experts. Registered parameters were the contact time, rearings, and short/long grooming episodes.

The free ToxTrac 2.98 software was used for off-line tracking of moving animals in all tests. The xld identification algorithm (Ssi.Rep.) was used. The algorithm used allowed us to track the rostral part of the body of a freely moving animal (which was important when evaluating social contacts). The following measurements were made automatically: the time spent in certain zones, the total distance travelled in certain zones, and the number and time of freezing episodes [53].

2.8. Statistical Analysis

The normality was checked by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov criterion (Basic Statistics, Descriptive Statistics section). The main method for assessing the significance of differences between the experimental and control groups was the Mann–Whitney U-test, and within-group effects were estimated by the Wilcoxon test. Bonferroni corrections were applied for between-group comparisons, under assumption of two independent behavioral domains (locomotor behavior and contact behavior). The differences were considered statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05. The main calculation of statistics took place using the Statistics 10.0 program, graphical images were created using Python 3.7.3 using the SciPy library.

3. Results

3.1. Elevated Plus Maze

The 1st EPM (n = 16 for KM rats, n = 16 for Wistar rats) test resulted in a similar between-strain difference as reported earlier by us [17] and others [54]. It is suggestive for a reduced exploratory drive and increased anxiety in KM rats. Namely, the total track length was shorter, as well as the open arms time; the number of rearings was lower and the number of facial grooming was higher, with a domination of freezing behavior in KM rats (see ).

The repeated, post-provocation EPM revealed that the horizontal activity of KM rats was increased as compared to the first testing and thus reached the level of control of the Wistar rats (Figure 3; ). Vertical activity was still deficient , and facial grooming enhanced in KM as compared to Wistar rats. The control rats did not change their locomotion between the EPM tests .

Figure 3. Behavior in the second (post-provocation) “elevated plus maze” test, comparison between the rat strains (upper panel, Wistar vs. KM) and KM subgroups (lower panel, sound provocation vs. sham provocation). Upper panel: (a) The total path travelled; (b) number of rearings t; (c) closed-to-open arm-entry ratio; (d) number of short (facial) grooming bouts; (e) total time of freezing behavior; (f) total number of entries in any arm. Lower panel: (g) The total path travelled; (h) number of rearings t; (i) closed-to-open arm-entry ratio; (j) number of short (facial) grooming bouts; (k) total time of freezing behavior; (l) total number of entries in any arm. Data are represented as the box plot and whiskers (indicating variability outside the upper and lower quartiles). Statistical significance is indicated as follows: *** p < 0.001, * p < 0.05.

The sound provocation per se did not exert a gross effect: horizontal and vertical locomotion did not differ between the subgroup of seizure-experienced and sham-provoked rats (Figure 3a,b; ). The AGS-experienced KM rats did not differ significantly from their sham-provoked strain mates .

3.2. Different Paradigms of “Social Preference/Social Novelty” Tests

3.3. Effect of Seizures’ Experience

The sound provocation did not evoke AGS in control rats or exert a gross behavioral effect in this cohort.

Surprisingly, no major difference between the “latent AGS” and «experienced AGS» subgroups of KM rats was seen in the post-provocation social tests. Existing social deficits in KM cohorts were not aggravated by the triple provocation of AGS. Instead, a tendency to activation was seen. Namely, a moderate decrease in anxiety levels was seen as the lowered number of freezing episodes and decreased facial grooming . Unexpectedly, the convulsive experience led to a tendency to enhance the contact behavior (see Section 3.2). Locomotion was not increased by the seizure experience. Instead, we observed a facilitation of locomotion by the repeated testing in KM but not Wistar rats (see below).

3.4. Effect of Repetitive Testing

The main behavioral tests (“elevated plus maze”, “socially enriched open field”, “three-chambered social novelty/social preference” tests) were performed twice, before and after the sound exposures (referred as the first and the second trials, or “pre-provocation”/“post-provocation”). For the control rats, we did not see a major difference between the first and second tests in the behavioral parameters registered . To overcome potential artifacts caused by the excessive freezing response and hypolocomotion, we used the percentages of time spent near the stimulus’ box and the percentages of path travelled around it .

As for the KM cohorts, the repeated testing helped to overcome the initial hypolocomotion (Figure 5a, Figure 6a, and Figure 7a; ), so that the two cohorts of epileptic rats almost reached the locomotor activity level of controls . Namely, no strain/subgroup difference was seen for the total distance travelled in the following tests: “elevated plus maze”, “three-chambered social preference test”, “socially enriched open field”.

Figure 5. Behavior in the “socially enriched open field” test, the second test (post-provocation). (a) An example of Wistar rat trajectory; (b) an example of KM rat trajectory. Upper panel: comparisons between the strains. (c) The total path length; number of short (d) and long (e) grooming; the number of short (f) and long (g) of snout-to-box contacts of the freely moving experimental animals; (h) the total freezing time. Lower panel: comparison between seizure-experienced and seizure-naïve KM subgroups. (i) The total path length, comparison between the KM subgroups. Number of short (j) and long (k) grooming; number of short (l) and long (m) snout-to-box contact, comparison between the KMs subgroups. Data are represented as a box plot and whiskers (indicating variability outside the upper and lower quartiles). Statistical significance is indicated as follows: *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.

Figure 6. Behavior in the “social preference/social novelty” tests, post-provocation, between groups comparison (Wistar vs. KM rats). The “social preference” session: (a) total path travelled; (b) number of short contacts; (c) number of long contacts; (d) total time of freezing s. The “social novelty” session: (e) total path travelled; (f) number of short contacts with “old” stimulus; (g) number of long contacts with “old” stimulus; (h) time of freezing response; (i) number of short contacts with “new” stimulus; (j) number of long contacts with “new” stimulus. Data are represented as a box plot and whiskers (indicating variability outside the upper and lower quartiles). Statistical significance is indicated as follows: *** p < 0.001, * p ≤ 0.05.

Figure 7. Behavior in the “social preference/social novelty” tests, post-provocation; comparison between the KM subgroups. The “social preference” session: (a) total path length; (b) number of short contacts; (c) number of long contacts. The “social novelty” session: (d) total path length, (e) number of short contacts with “familiar” stimulus; (f) number of long contacts with “familiar” stimulus; (g) number of short contacts with “unfamiliar” stimulus; (h) number of long contacts with “unfamiliar” stimulus. Data are represented as a box plot and whiskers (indicating variability outside the upper and lower quartiles). No significant difference was seen between the subgroups.

The deficits in social contacts generally remained, seen in the decreased numbers of prolonged contacts with the stimulus (“socially enriched open field”, see Section 3.2.2; “social novelty” test, all p < 0.01, see Section 3.2.3).

3.5. Two Object Novel Object Recognition Test

Novel object recognition tests are used to assess investigatory drive and memory in laboratory rodents. We used the same apparatus as in the previous experiments (see Section 3.2.3), but without the partition, to check interaction with inanimate objects, in the same environmental context as the social interactions were observed. KM (n = 12) and Wistar (n = 16) demonstrated only a minor difference in the test: KM more often interacted with the new object (Figure 8(a.2,b.2)). The number of active contacts with the new object (i.e., touching with paws, transferring in teeth, or gnawing the objects) was higher in KM rats . The results also showed that the rats of the KM strain were not frightened by the novelty of small inanimate objects. They kept interacting with the object, while the control Wistar rats lost their interest quite soon.

Figure 8. The “two objects novel object test”, between strain comparisons. Investigation of new and familiar objects. (a.1) Contacts with a new object; (a.2) active contacts with a new object; (b.1) contact with an “old” object; (b.2) Active contact with an “old” object. Data are represented as a box plot and whiskers (indicating variability outside the upper and lower quartiles). Statistical significance is indicated as follows: *** p < 0.001, * p < 0.05.

3.6. Social Dominance “Tube” Test

Contact motivation deficits might be a result of a social defeat experience or a lack of social dominance. To test this, we run the test on a dominant behavior, so called “social dominance in a tube”. According to the results, KM rats won the test more frequently . It should indicate a greater level of dominance in the KM, as compared to the Wistar rats. At the same time, the latency to win was significantly longer in KM rats (Figure 9a; ). A more detailed behavioral analysis showed that the KM rats made a fewer number of pushes during the sparring and fewer retreat trials to escape (Figure 9b,d). KM prevented the opponents from moving forward through the pipe. At the same time, the number of forward propulsions was similar for the both groups (Figure 9c). Thus, Wistar and KM groups demonstrated different behavioral strategies in TSD. Wistar rats preferred an active strategy, as evidenced by the low latency to win and a larger number of pushes and retreat trials. On the contrary, KM rats chose a rather passive strategy characterized by slow movements, a lack of retreats, and gradual progression forward by the time of retreat trials of Wistars.

Figure 9. Social dominance test, between strain comparisons. (a) Latency to win; (b) number of pushes; (c) number of forward propulsions; (d) number of retreats. Data are represented as a box plot and whiskers (indicating variability outside the upper and lower quartiles). Statistical significance is indicated as follows: *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01.

Thus, consistent social contact deficits were observed in KM rats, subjected to a battery of behavioral test. The active phase of epileptogenesis was started by a triple provocation of audiogenic seizures, and the presumed behavioral changes were monitored post-provocation. As a result of seizure provocations, as well as of multiple experimental sessions, we observed a mild behavioral activation in KM rats. Namely, hypolocomotor traits in KM rats were rescued up to the control level, together with a decrease in anxious behavior. Contact deficits were consistently seen both before the AGS provocation as well as post-provocation. Control groups of Wistar rats did not majorly change their behavior during the course of experimentations.

4. Discussion

Epilepsy-related social and intellectual regress is a major concern for ASD patients [3]. To study this phenomenon experimentally, we focused on the start point of active epileptogenesis in an animal model of comorbid epilepsy and behavioral deficits in KM rats. We observed that hypolocomotion might be dissociated from the contact deficits in KM rats and that the social deficiency persisted during the multiple tests.

The hypolocomotor trait of KM rats can be normalized by multiple exposures to the test. Namely, KM rats displayed evident hypolocomotion in all the first (i.e., pre-provocation) tests (namely, EPM, “socially enriched open field”, “social preference/social novelty”, see Section 3.1 and Section 3.2., but not in all the post-convulsive tests. The feature contrasted with the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)-like hyperlocomotor phenotype of other animal models of epilepsy comorbid with social deficits: prenatal valproate loading [55] and fast/slow kindling rat strain [56]. At the same time, the observed hypolocomotion was in line with that reported for a maternal immune activation model of ASD [57]. Remarkably, profound neuroinflammation also plays a role in the pathogenesis of audiogenic epilepsy in KM rats, since a number of pro-inflammatory cytokines were affected by sound exposure in KM rats [22]. It is interesting to check whether hypolocomotion would be seen in other animal models of neuroinflammation-associated ASD.

The locomotor response in KM rats might be restored almost to the normal level by an extra-long habituation to the experimental conditions, as seen from the current results (in the second trials of the “elevated plus maze”, “three-chambered social preference test”). It is possible that longer repetitive social exposures would normalize contacts with conspecifics, in its turn. In our design, the same tests were separated by 6 weeks; it remains a question whether a quicker habituation would be possible for these rats. Anyway, the habituation to the experimental set-ups in KM rats was obviously longer than the regular 20–30 min sessions needed for normal rats. The feature is strongly suggestive for a disadaptation to environmental novelty in KM rats.

The experimental design allows discrimination between the effect of seizures/sound stress and the habituation effects, by the comparison of sham-provoked and sound-provoked groups. Expectably, control Wistar rats did not show any major effects from the sound stress administration . Surprisingly, there was only a minimal effect of the seizures’ experience in the KM rats. The severe but rare seizures exert rather a mild activation in KM rats, seen as decreased bouts of freezing and short grooming (; Section 3.3), as well as a tendency toward contact activation (Figure 7f–h and ). No other behavioral consequences of the triple AGS provocation were seen in our experiments.

The tendency to AGS-related mild contact activation might partly parallel a facilitating effect of the electroconvulsive therapy. The method is widely used in clinical practice to treat major depression, Parkinson’s disease, and/or schizophrenia [57,58,59,60]. The mechanisms are not fully understood, but at least partly it acts via an activation of brain aminergic metabolism [61,62,63]. Brain monoamines are imbalanced in KM rats [12,21,23], so one may hypothesize a facilitated turnover of brain amines as a putative mechanism for the observed behavioral activation. It would be irrational to recommend electroconvulsive therapies to treat ASD; however, there might be a mechanism to overcome the hypolocomotion and contact deficits.

The KM rats demonstrated a lack of social motivation in all the experimental setups used. The three-chambered “social novelty” test (Section 3.2.3, Figure 6g,h) appears to be the most sensitive one. We might hypothesize that this particular setup still leaves a possibility for spatial behavior but does not allow escaping the proximity of encaged “guest” rats. Also, it produced a high spatial density of conspecifics. It is known from clinical practice that ASD patients have particular difficulties in multiple social interactions at a time.

As it was shown previously, the contact behavior of freely moving rodents (rats or mice) did not depend on the locomotion of the encaged stimulus conspecific inside the container [34]. However, the contact behavior implies individuality in the interactions, putatively involving audial, tactile, and/or olfactory modalities. The contact behavior of test rats depended on a space available for interaction, as well as on ability to touch and smell the guest rats [39]. A close spatial confinement of interacting rats led to decreased contact motivation, while their partition by barred walls preserved it. Tactile interactions were essential for social reward, whereas visual contacting via a glass screen was not [39]. In our experiments, perforated walls allowed the olfactory inspections of all the containers, which were unified in smelling the bedding compound from the guest rats’ home cages. So, the only presumed difference between the containers was a presence of an encaged conspecific. The communication windows (1.5 cm * 8 cm holes, Section 2.3.2) ensured a possibility for tactile (snout-to-snout touches) and audial contacts, thought to be the main factors for establishing a social affiliation in rats, e.g., [39,40,41]. Audial communications had been recorded and remained to be published; here we measured social approaches only by the parameters of snout-to-box contacts.

A distinction between curiosity and social drive can be made assessing the contact time [34]. Short (less than 6 s) bouts of investigation reflect curiosity per se, while longer well-developed contacts relate to communicative behavior. Thus, our results show both a lack of curiosity and a deficiency in communicative drive in KM rats (Section 3.2).

The Novel Object Recognition (NOR) task is used to assess cognitive abilities, in particular recognition memory, in models of CNS disorders in rodents [47]. This test is based on the spontaneous tendency of rodents to spend more time studying a new object than a familiar one. The choice to study a new object reflects the use of learning and recognition memory. In our experiments, KM rats displayed decreased contacts of animated objects (i.e., boxes with a conspecific), but spent a longer time inspecting inanimate small objects and interacting with them (Figure 8(a2,b2)). It points to a social specificity of the observed investigatory deficits in KM rats.

In the social dominance test, KM rats demonstrated a poor behavioral repertoire, choosing a passive progression strategy. However, in this test, it is just the strategy that turned out to be more advantageous, since it did not allow the opponent pushing such a rat out. KM rats could move forward without pushing out the opponent, but rather took up a space after its active retreat. Wistar rats preferred to escape confrontations, and it seemed as they voluntarily left the pipe. Thus, KM rats did not show active dominance, but also did not allow dominating them. Similar results have been reported recently in the “social dominance in a tube test” for high-ranking Syngap+/Δ-GAP rats (heterozygous for the C2/GAP domain deletion, with ASD-like traits). The Syngap+/Δ-GAP individuals won significantly more competitions against high-rank wild type rats [64]. We can conclude that the consistent social deficits seen in KM rats (Section 3.2) were not attributable to a putative social defeat experience or submissive social traits.

An obvious limitation of the present work is the lack of female subjects in the study. Although ASD has a profound male prevalence in clinical practice [65], the female ASD also needs extensive attention of basic and clinical scientists. However, the official breeder of the KM rat strain, Biological Faculty of Moscow State University, restricts a dissemination of female KM individuals, for sake of the guaranteed genetic purity. So, experimentations in female KM rats, as well as early ontogenetic studies, are not feasible for external teams.

In the present experiments, we did not distinguish clearly between the sensory modalities engaged in social affiliation and its disruption. Visual inputs seem to be minimal since we used dark perforated containers with gap-like communication windows (Section 3.2.2 and Section 3.2.3). Presumably, the tactile contacts provided the core component of social reward in rats [39,66]. But it is also possible that reciprocity of tactile or audial contacts is of importance. To clarify this point, experimentation with toy rats or a deactivation of stimuli rats by a light narcosis would be useful.

A clear advantage of the study is the experimental proof that severe but still rare seizures do not cause an obligatory decay in sociability, which would be a strong concern for many caregivers of ASD children. Moving further and applying more seizure provocations, we may find the time point of a seizure-related regress in ability to develop social contacts. This would allow dissecting possible neurodegenerative mechanisms of subserving a social withdrawal; thus, this knowledge, accumulated in epileptology, might be of benefit for the understanding ASD-related neurological issues. For this purpose, we need various animal models in order to apply seizure provocations in a controlled way and monitor emotional and motivational changes, paralleling the time course of epilepsy.

In experimentations with laboratory animals, it is important to level out possible contextual artifacts by using a battery of behavioral tests [29]. Multidimensional ethological analysis, applied to behavioral components of social deficits in laboratory animals, and a concise translation of the knowledge to a broader context of human neuropathology would bridge a way to find effective treatments for ASD-related clinical issues.

The obtained results confirmed and expanded our previous conclusions about social deficiency in KM rats. One of the oldest inbred models of genetic epilepsy has translational potential for studying the neurophysiology and pharmacology of ASD-like social deficiencies. A special clinical domain of the autistic spectrum, characterized by hypolocomotion and excessive freezing [67], can be modeled in the KM strain. It is important to note that anxiety and locomotor deficits could be normalized by extra-long habituation and/or repeated testing; the same was not observed for social contacts deficiency in these rats. Thus, hypolocomotion was experimentally dissociated from contact deficiency. It means that we need more than just an environmental habituation to treat social contact deficits.

Importantly, severe but rare audiogenic convulsions, as well as sound provocations per se, did not aggravate contact deficits in the rats tested. This suggests that between the seizure onset and an impending seizure-related social regress, there is a time window to find an effective anti-epileptic therapy.

References

  1. Liu, X.; Sun, X.; Sun, C.; Zou, M.; Chen, Y.; Huang, J.; Wu, L.; Chen, W.-X. Prevalence of Epilepsy in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Autism 2022, 26, 33–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Besag, F. Epilepsy in Patients with Autism: Links, Risks and Treatment Challenges. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2017, 14, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Gadow, K.D.; Perlman, G.; Weber, R.J. Parent-Reported Developmental Regression in Autism: Epilepsy, IQ, Schizophrenia Spectrum Symptoms, and Special Education. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 918–926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Zahra, A.; Wang, Y.; Wang, Q.; Wu, J. Shared Etiology in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Epilepsy with Functional Disability. Behav. Neurol. 2022, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Brooks-Kayal, A. Epilepsy and autism spectrum disorders: Are there common developmental mechanisms? Brain Dev. 2010, 32, 731–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Sierra-Arregui TLlorente, J.; Giménez Minguez, P.; Tønnesen, J.; Peñagarikano, O. Neurobiological Mechanisms of Autism Spectrum Disorder and Epilepsy, Insights from Animal Models//Neuroscience. Pergamon 2020, 445, 69–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Schneider, T.; Przewłocki, R. Behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid: Animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005, 30, 80–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gilby, K.L. A new rat model for vulnerability to epilepsy and autism spectrum disorders. Epilepsia 2008, 49, 108–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Jung, S.; Seo, J.S.; Kim, B.S.; Lee, D.; Jung, K.H.; Chu, K.; Lee, S.K.; Jeon, D. Social deficits in the AY-9944 mouse model of atypical absence epilepsy. Behav. Brain Res. 2013, 236, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wang, X.; Guo, Z.; Mei, D.; Zhang, Y.; Zhao, S.; Hu, S.; Luo, S.; Wang, Q.; Gao, C. The GluN2B-Trp373 NMDA Receptor Variant is Associated with Autism-, Epilepsy-Related Phenotypes and Reduces NMDA Receptor Currents in Rats. Neurochem. Res. 2022, 47, 1588–1597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Vergnes, M.; Marescaux, C.; Boehrer, A.; Depaulis, A. Are rats with genetic absence epilepsy behaviorally impaired? Epilepsy Res. 1991, 9, 97–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Sarkisova, K.Y.; Midzianovskaia, I.S.; Kulikov, M.A. Depressive-like behavioral alterations and c-fos expression in the dopaminergic brain regions in WAG/Rij rats with genetic absence epilepsy. Behav. Brain Res. 2003, 144, 211–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Leo, A.; Citraro, R.; Tallarico, M.; Iannone, M.; Fedosova, E.; Nesci, V.; De Sarro, G.; Sarkisova, K.; Russo, E. Cognitive impairment in the WAG/Rij rat absence model is secondary to absence seizures and depressive-like behavior. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2019, 94, 109652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Garcia-Cairasco, N.; Umeoka, E.H.L.; Cortes de Oliveira, J.A. The Wistar Audiogenic Rat (WAR) Strain and Its Contributions to Epileptology and Related Comorbidities: History and Perspectives. Epilepsy Behav. 2017, 71, 250–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Washington, J.; Kumar, U.; Medel-Matus, J.-S.; Shin, D.; Sankar, R.; Mazarati, A. Cytokine-Dependent Bidirectional Connection between Impaired Social Behavior and Susceptibility to Seizures Associated with Maternal Immune Activation in Mice. Epilepsy Behav. 2015, 50, 40–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Hirota, T.; Veenstra-VanderWeele, J.; Hollander, E.; Kishi, T. Antiepileptic medications in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 948–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rebik, A.A.; Riga, V.D.; Smirnov, K.S.; Sysoeva, O.V.; Midzyanovskaya, I.S. Social Behavioral Deficits in Krushinsky-Molodkina Rats, an Animal Model of Audiogenic Epilepsy. J. Pers. Med. 2022, 12, 2062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Semiokhina, A.F.; Rat’kin, A.E.; Fedotova, I.B.; Kuznetsova, L.M.; Kostyna, Z.A.; Sotskaia, M.N.; Chebykina, L.I. A model of audiogenic epilepsy--rats of the Krushinskiĭ-Molodkina strain. Proof of genetic homogeneity. Zhurnal Vyss. Nervn. Deiatelnosti Im. IP Pavlova. 1996, 46, 592–596. [Google Scholar]
  19. Poletaeva, I.I.; Surina, N.M.; Kostina, Z.A.; Perepelkina, O.V.; Fedotova, I.B. The Krushinsky-Molodkina rat strain: The study of audiogenic epilepsy for 65years. Epilepsy Behav. 2017, 71, 130–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Harbachova, E.L.; Chernigovskaya, E.V.; Glazova, M.V.; Nikitina, L.S. Audiogenic Kindling Activates Expression of Vasopressin in the Hypothalamus of Krushinsky-Molodkina Rats Genetically Prone to Reflex Epilepsy. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2022, 32, e12846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Tsyba, E.T.; Midzyanovskaya, I.S.; Birioukova, L.M.; Tuomisto, L.M.; van Luijtelaar, G.; Abbasova, K.R. Striatal patchwork of D1-like and D2-like receptors binding densities in rats with genetic audiogenic and absence epilepsies. Diagnostics 2023, 13, 587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Surina, N.M.; Fedotova, I.B.; Nikolaev, G.M.; Grechenko, V.V.; Gankovskaya, L.V.; Ogurtsova, A.D.; Poletaeva, I.I. Neuroinflammation in Pathogenesis of Audiogenic Epilepsy: Altered Proinflammatory Cytokine Levels in the Rats of Krushinsky–Molodkina Seizure-Prone Strain. Biochemistry 2023, 88, 481–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Litvinova, S.A.; Voronina, T.A.; Kudrin, V.S.; Narkevich, V.B.; Surina, N.M.; Poletaeva, I.I.; Fedotova, I.B. The Role of Brain Monoamines in the Formation of Audiogenic Myoclonic Seizures in Krushinsky–Molodkina Rats. Neurochem. J. 2023, 17, 84–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Acikgoz, B.; Dalkiran, B.; Dayi, A. An overview of the currency and usefulness of behavioral tests used from past to present to assess anxiety, social behavior and depression in rats and mice. Behav. Process. 2022, 200, 104670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Morimoto, K.; Fahnestock, M.; Racine, R.J. Kindling and Status Epilepticus Models of Epilepsy: Rewiring the Brain. Prog. Neurobiol. 2004, 73, 1–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Adamec, R.; Young, B. Neuroplasticity in Specific Limbic System Circuits May Mediate Specific Kindling Induced Changes in Animal Affect—Implications for Understanding Anxiety Associated with Epilepsy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000, 24, 705–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kulikov, A.A.; Naumova, A.A.; Dorofeeva, N.A.; Ivlev, A.P.; Glazova, M.V.; Chernigovskaya, E.V. Dynamics of Neurodegeneration in the Hippocampus of Krushinsky-Molodkina Rats Correlates with the Progression of Limbic Seizures. Epilepsy Behav. 2022, 134, 108846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Valentim-Lima, E.; Oliveira, J.A.C.; Antunes-Rodrigues, J.; Reis, L.C.; Garcia-Cairasco, N.; Mecawi, A.S. Neuroendocrine Changes in the Hypothalamic-neurohypophysial System in the Wistar Audiogenic Rat (WAR) Strain Submitted to Audiogenic Kindling. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2021, 33, e12975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Jabarin, R.; Netser, S.; Wagner, S. Beyond the Three-Chamber Test: Toward a Multimodal and Objective Assessment of Social Behavior in Rodents. Mol. Autism. 2022, 13, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Winslow, J.T. Mouse Social Recognition and Preference. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. 2003, 22, 8–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Endo, N.; Makinodan, M.; Somayama, N.; Komori, T.; Kishimoto, T.; Nishi, M. Characterization of Behavioral Phenotypes in the BTBR T+ Itpr3tf/J Mouse Model of Autism Spectrum Disorder under Social Housing Conditions Using the Multiple Animal Positioning System. Exp. Anim. 2019, 68, 319–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kiyokawa, Y.; Kuroda, N.; Takeuchi, Y. The Strain of Unfamiliar Conspecifics Affects Stress Identification in Rats. Behav. Process. 2022, 201, 104714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Zou, M.; Liu, Y.; Xie, S.; Wang, L.; Li, D.; Li, L.; Wang, F.; Zhang, Y.; Xia, W.; Sun, C.; et al. Alterations of the endocannabinoid system and its therapeutic potential in autism spectrum disorder. Open Biol. 2021, 11, 200306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Netser, S.; Meyer, A.; Magalnik, H.; Zylbertal, A.; de la Zerda, S.H.; Briller, M.; Bizer, A.; Grinevich, V.; Wagner, S. Distinct dynamics of social motivation drive differential social behavior in laboratory rat and mouse strains. Nat. Commun. 2020, 11, 5908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Castro, G.P.; de Castro Medeiros, D.; de Oliveira Guarnieri, L.; Mourão FA, G.; Pinto HP, P.; Pereira, G.S.; Moraes, M.F.D. Wistar audiogenic rats display abnormal behavioral traits associated with artificial selection for seizure susceptibility. Epilepsy Behav. 2017, 71, 243–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Zhang, Y.-F.; Janke, E.; Bhattarai, J.P.; Wesson, D.W.; Ma, M. Self-Directed Orofacial Grooming Promotes Social Attraction in Mice via Chemosensory Communication. iScience 2022, 25, 104284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Zhang-James, Y.; Yang, L.; Middleton, F.A.; Yang, L.; Patak, J.; Faraone, S.V. Autism-Related Behavioral Phenotypes in an Inbred Rat Substrain. Behav. Brain Res. 2014, 269, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Shirenova, S.D.; Khlebnikova, N.N.; Krupina, N.A. Changes in Sociability and Preference for Social Novelty in Female Rats in Prolonged Social Isolation. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 2023, 53, 103–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kummer, K.; Klement, S.; Eggart, V.; Mayr, M.J.; Saria, A.; Zernig, G. Conditioned place preference for social interaction in rats: Contribution of sensory components. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Wöhr, M.; Schwarting, R.K. Ultrasonic communication in rats: Can playback of 50-kHz calls induce approach behavior? PLoS ONE 2007, 2, e1365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Schirmer, A.; McGlone, F. Editorial overview: Affective touch: Neurobiology and function. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2022, 45, 101129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Haratizadeh, S.; Parvan, M.; Mohammadi, S.; Shabani, M.; Nozari, M. An overview of modeling and behavioral assessment of autism in the rodent. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2021, 81, 221–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Korte, S.M.; De Boer, S.F. A robust animal model of state anxiety: Fear-potentiated behaviour in the elevated plus-maze. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2003, 463, 163–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Beery, A.K.; Shambaugh, K.L. Comparative assessment of familiarity/novelty preferences in rodents. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 648830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Cao, W.Y.; Hu, Z.L.; Xu, Y.; Zhang, W.J.; Huang, F.L.; Qiao, X.Q.; Cui, Y.H.; Wan, W.; Wang, X.Q.; Liu, D.; et al. Role of early environmental enrichment on the social dominance tube test at adulthood in the rat. Psychopharmacology 2017, 234, 3321–3334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Fan, Z.; Zhu, H.; Zhou, T.; Wang, S.; Wu, Y.; Hu, H. Using the tube test to measure social hierarchy in mice. Nat. Protoc. 2019, 14, 819–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Ennaceur, A.; Michalikova, S.; Chazot, P.L. Do rats really express neophobia towards novel objects? Experimental evidence from exposure to novelty and to an object recognition task in an open space and an enclosed space. Behav. Brain Res. 2009, 197, 417–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Kuznetsova, G.D. Audiogenic seizures in rats of different genetic strains. Zhurn. Vissh. Nervn. Dejat. 1998, 48, 143–152. [Google Scholar]
  49. Midzyanovskaya, I.S.; Kuznetsova, G.D.; Vinogradova, L.V.; Shatskova, A.B.; Coenen, A.M.; van Luijtelaar, G. Mixed forms of epilepsy in a subpopulation of WAG/Rij rats. Epilepsy Behav. 2004, 5, 655–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Vinogradova, L.V.; Kuznetsova, G.D.; Coenen, A.M.L. Audiogenic seizures associated with a cortical spreading depression wave suppress spike-wave discharges in rats. Physiol. Behav. 2005, 86, 554–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Vinogradova, L.V. Audiogenic kindling and secondary subcortico-cortical epileptogenesis: Behavioral correlates and electrographic features. Epilepsy Behav. 2017, 71, 142–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Jobe, P.C.; Picchioni, A.L.; Chin, L. Role of brain norepinephrine in audiogenic seizures in the rat. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1973, 184, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  53. Rodriguez, A.; Zhang, H.; Klaminder, J.; Brodin, T.; Andersson, P.L.; Andersson, M. ToxTrac: A fast and robust software for tracking organisms. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2018, 9, 460–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Sarkisova, K.Y.; Fedotova, I.B.; Surina, N.M.; Nikolaev, G.M.; Perepelkina, O.V.; Kostina, Z.A.; Poletaeva, I.I. Genetic background contributes to the co-morbidity of anxiety and depression with audiogenic seizure propensity and responses to fluoxetine treatment. Epilepsy Behav. 2017, 68, 95–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Ergaz, Z.; Weinstein-Fudim, L.; Ornoy, A. Genetic and non-genetic animal models for autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Reprod. Toxicol. 2016, 64, 116–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Gilby, K.L.; O’Brien, T.J. Epilepsy, autism, and neurodevelopment: Kindling a shared vulnerability? Epilepsy Behav. 2013, 26, 370–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Usui, N.; Kobayashi, H.; Shimada, S. Neuroinflammation and Oxidative Stress in the Pathogenesis of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2023, 24, 5487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Cumper, S.K.; Ahle, G.M.; Liebman, L.S.; Kellner, C.H. Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) in Parkinson’s disease: ECS and dopamine enhancement. J. ECT 2014, 30, 122–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Nikisch, G.; Mathé, A.A. CSF monoamine metabolites and neuropeptides in depressed patients before and after electroconvulsive therapy. Eur. Psychiatry 2008, 23, 356–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Saijo, T.; Takano, A.; Suhara, T.; Arakawa, R.; Okumura, M.; Ichimiya, T.; Ito, H.; Okubo, Y. Effect of electroconvulsive therapy on 5-HT1A receptor binding in patients with depression: A PET study with [11C]WAY 100635. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010, 13, 785–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Goto, S.; Terao, T.; Hoaki, N.; Wang, Y.; Tsuchiyama, K.; Araki, Y.; Kohno, K. Is serotonergic function associated with the antidepressant effects of modified-electroconvulsive therapy? J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 136, 1062–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Swartz, C.M. A mechanism of seizure induction by electricity and its clinical implications. J. ECT 2014, 30, 94–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Njau, S.; Joshi, S.H.; Espinoza, R.; Leaver, A.M.; Vasavada, M.; Marquina, A.; Woods, R.P.; Narr, K.L. Neurochemical correlates of rapid treatment response to electroconvulsive therapy in patients with major depression. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2017, 42, 6–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Harris, E.; Myers, H.; Saxena, K.; Mitchell-Heggs, R.; Kind, P.; Chattarji, S.; Morris, R.G.M. Experiential modulation of social dominance in a SYNGAP1 rat model of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2021, 54, 7733–7748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Werling, D.M.; Geschwind, D.H. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2013, 26, 146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Fritz, M.; El Rawas, R.; Salti, A.; Klement, S.; Bardo, M.T.; Kemmler, G.; Zernig, G. Reversal of cocaine-conditioned place preference and mesocorticolimbic Zif268 expression by social interaction in rats. Addict. Biol. 2011, 16, 273–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Posserud, M.; Hysing, M.; Helland, W.; Gillberg, C.; Lundervold, A.J. Autism traits: The importance of “co-morbid” problems for impairment and contact with services. Data from the Bergen Child Study. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 72, 275–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Related Suggestion
Analysis of Adverse Drug Reactions in Pediatric Patients with Epilepsy: An Intensive Pharmacovigilance Study
Clinical
Epilepsy is a chronic neurological disease characterized by the presence of spontaneous seizures, with a higher incidence in the pediatric population. Anti-seizure medication (ASM) may produce adverse drug reactions (ADRs) with an elevated frequency and a high severity. Thus, the objective of the present study was to analyze, through intensive pharmacovigilance over 112 months, the ADRs produced by valproic acid (VPA), oxcarbazepine (OXC), phenytoin (PHT), and levetiracetam (LEV), among others, administered to monotherapy or polytherapy for Mexican hospitalized pediatric epilepsy patients. A total of 1034 patients were interviewed; 315 met the inclusion criteria, 211 patients presented ADRs, and 104 did not. A total of 548 ASM-ADRs were identified, and VPA, LEV, and PHT were the main culprit drugs. The most frequent ADRs were drowsiness, irritability, and thrombocytopenia, and the main systems affected were hematologic, nervous, and dermatologic. LEV and OXC caused more nonsevere ADRs, and PHT caused more severe ADRs. The risk analysis showed an association between belonging to the younger groups and polytherapy with ADR presence and between polytherapy and malnutrition with severe ADRs. In addition, most of the severe ADRs were preventable, and most of the nonsevere ADRs were nonpreventable.
144
0
The Effects of Music-Based Interventions for Pain and Anxiety Management during Vaginal Labour and Caesarean Delivery: A Systematic Review and Narrative Synthesis of Randomised Controlled Trials
Clinical
Music-based interventions are not physically invasive, they usually have minimal side effects, and they are increasingly being implemented during the birthing process for pain and anxiety relief. The aim of this systematic review is to summarise and evaluate published, randomised controlled trials (RCTs) assessing the effects of music-based interventions for pain and anxiety management during vaginal labour and caesarean delivery. Following the PRISMA guidelines, a systematic search of the literature was conducted using: PsychInfo (Ovid), PubMed, and Web of Science. Studies were included in the review if they were RCTs that assessed the effects of music on pain and anxiety during vaginal and caesarean delivery by human mothers. A narrative synthesis was conducted on 28 identified studies with a total of 2835 participants. Most, but not all, of the included studies assessing music-based interventions resulted in reduced anxiety and pain during vaginal and caesarean delivery. Music as part of a comprehensive treatment strategy, participant-selected music, music coupled with another therapy, and relaxing/instrumental music was specifically useful for reducing light to moderate pain and anxiety. Music-based interventions show promising effects in mitigating pain and anxiety in women during labour. However, the long-term effects of these interventions are unclear.
144
0
Aflatoxin B1 Exposure Aggravates Neurobehavioral Deficits and Immune Dysfunctions of Th1, Th9, Th17, Th22, and T Regulatory Cell-Related Transcription Factor Signaling in the BTBR T+Itpr3tf/J Mouse Model of Autism
Clinical
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disease characterized by impaired communication, reciprocal social interactions, restricted sociability deficits, and stereotyped behavioral patterns. Environmental factors and genetic susceptibility have been implicated in an increased risk of ASD. Aflatoxin B1 (AFB1) is a typical contaminant of food and feed that causes severe immune dysfunction in humans and animals. Nevertheless, the impact of ASD on behavioral and immunological responses has not been thoroughly examined. To investigate this phenomenon, we subjected BTBR T+Itpr3tf/J (BTBR) mice to AFB1 and evaluated their marble-burying and self-grooming behaviors and their sociability. The exposure to AFB1 resulted in a notable escalation in marble-burying and self-grooming activities while concurrently leading to a decline in social contacts. In addition, we investigated the potential molecular mechanisms that underlie the impact of AFB1 on the production of Th1 (IFN-γ, STAT1, and T-bet), Th9 (IL-9 and IRF4), Th17 (IL-17A, IL-21, RORγT, and STAT3), Th22 (IL-22, AhR, and TNF-α), and T regulatory (Treg) (IL-10, TGF-β1, and FoxP3) cells in the spleen. This was achieved using RT-PCR and Western blot analyses to assess mRNA and protein expression in brain tissue. The exposure to AFB1 resulted in a significant upregulation of various immune-related factors, including IFN-γ, STAT1, T-bet, IL-9, IRF4, IL-17A, IL-21, RORγ, STAT3, IL-22, AhR, and TNF-α in BTBR mice. Conversely, the production of IL-10, TGF-β1, and FoxP3 by CD4+ T cells was observed to be downregulated. Exposure to AFB1 demonstrated a notable rise in Th1/Th9/Th22/Th17 levels and a decrease in mRNA and protein expression of Treg. The results above underscore the significance of AFB1 exposure in intensifying neurobehavioral and immunological abnormalities in BTBR mice, hence indicating the necessity for a more comprehensive investigation into the contribution of AFB1 to the development of ASD.
100
0
Comments 0
Please to post a comment~
Loading...
Likes
Send-Pen
Favorites
Comment